The Importance of Fluency and Automaticity for Efficient Reading
The Importance of Automaticity and Fluency For Efficient
Reading Comprehension
by Pamela E. Hook and Sandra D. Jones
Reprinted with permission from the International Dyslexia
Association quarterly newsletter, Perspectives, Winter, 2002, vol. 28, no. 1,
pages 9-14. IDA website: http://www.interdys.org.
My abridgement:
What are the relationships among phonemic awareness, phonics and
orthographic reading?
The importance of background knowledge for comprehension also
increases. Fluent reading at this point is essential.
Automatic reading involves the development of strong orthographic
representations, which allows fast and accurate identification of whole words
made up of specific letter patterns. English orthography is generally alphabetic
in nature and initially word identification is based on the application of
phonic word attack strategies (letter-sound association). These word attack
strategies are in turn based on the development of phonemic awareness, which is
necessary to learn how to map speech to print. It is important to keep in mind
that prior to the stage where children read orthographically, they apply
alphabetic strategies to analyze words (Frith, 1985).
Figure 1: Relationships Among Phonemic Awareness, Phonics, and Sight Word
Recognition Skills.
Figure 1 illustrates the relationships among the processes involved in word
identification. The bottom of the figure depicts a strong base in phonemic
awareness upon which word identification skills are built. There is, however, a
reciprocal relationship between the development of phonemic awareness and the
development of phonic word attack strategies. As the child becomes more familiar
with letters, phonemic awareness also improves. Ultimately, strong underlying
orthographic patterns begin to emerge.
WORD LEVEL-AUTOMATICITY
How do we develop this automatic, orthographic reading ability in our
students?
In the beginning - the six syllable types
One of
the most powerful tools to begin instruction in this area is using the visual
patterns inherent in the six syllable types (Steere, Peck and Kahn, 1988.) (See
Figure 2.)
Figure 2. The Six Syllable Types
1. closed
-not
(closed in by a consonant - vowel makes its
short sound.
2. open - no
(ends in a vowel -
vowel makes its long sound
3. silent e -
note
(ends in vowel consonant e - vowel makes its
long sound
4. Vowel combination - nail
(the
two vowels together make a sound)
5. r controlled -
bird
(contains a vowel plus 4 - vowel sound is changed)
6.
consonant- l -e - table
(at the end of a
word) |
It is these letter (orthographic) patterns that signal vowel pronunciation. Anna Gillingham when she
incorporated the Phonetic Word Cards activity into the Orton-Gillingham lesson
plan (Gillingham and Stillman, 1997). This activity involves having the student
practice reading words (and some nonwords) on cards as wholes beginning with
simple syllables and moving systematically through the syllable types to complex
syllables and two-syllable words. The words are divided into groups that
correspond to the specific sequence of skills being taught.
Accent
In addition to acquiring phonic word attack strategies, prosodic features at
the word level such as stress on syllables are important. At times, poor readers
can accurately decode a word but true recognition of the word eludes them
because they have not correctly accented one of the syllables. Dyslexic students
often have difficulty hearing the accented syllables in a word, so teachers
should first determine if a student is able to discriminate and identify through
listening alone. If a student cannot hear differences, lessons should begin with
listening practice and then move to oral production. Visual and
tactile/kinesthetic strategies can be incorporated with listening if necessary.
A
teacher can begin to practice listening for and producing accented syllables
using the alphabet. The alphabet is presented in pairs and one letter
of the pair is accented until a student is successful. The teacher begins
presenting the accent only on the first letter until the student is consistent
and then presents the accent only on the second letter. When a student can do
both of these successfully, the teacher can then present mixed pairs to practice
(e.g., A'B C'D EF or GH' IJ' KL' or
M'N O P' Q'R). Instruction should move as quickly as
possible from using the alphabet to using real words. The teacher can begin with
names to practice listening for accented syllables (e.g.,
Court' ney, Michelle', Ty ler, Je
sus').
Training and Linking the Orthographic Processor
Although most students learn to apply the rules of phonic word attack
strategies and later of prefix, stem, and suffix through multisensory,
structured systematic teaching techniques and become quite accurate in their
reading, many remain slow and laborious. They have trouble moving to the next
level of automatic orthographic reading and thus comprehension suffers.
Approaches have been developed that use a variety of repeated reading
strategies to strengthen these orthographic images. The automatic recognition of
single graphemes is a critical first step to the development of the letter
patterns that make up words or word parts (Adams, 1990). English orthography is
made up of four basic kinds of words: 1) regular for reading and spelling (e.g.,
mat, sprint; 2) regular for reading but not for spelling (e.g. boat,
rain - could be spelled "bote" or "rane" respectively); 3) rule based (e.g.,
planning - doubling rule, baking - drop e rule); and 4) irregular
(e.g. beauty - it should be noted that most parts of an "irregular" word
are actually regular and only the irregular part needs to be specifically
addressed).
Students must learn to recognize all four types of words automatically in
order to be effective readers; thus, techniques for developing strong
orthographic representations for all types of words are essential. Extensive
opportunity for repeated practice in pattern recognition is often necessary. Other ways of emphasizing orthography would be card sorts
where students sort different syllable types into appropriate categories as
quickly as possible and then say the vowel sounds of each syllable type as
quickly as possible.
Single word level word drills - regular and irregular words
Once these orthographic signals have become automatically recognized, the
students complete speed drills in which they read lists of isolated words with
contrasting vowel sounds that are signalled by the syllable type. For example,
six to eight closed syllable and vowel-consonant-e words containing the vowel
a are arranged randomly on pages containing about 12 lines and read for
one minute. Individual goals are established and charts are kept of the number
of words read correctly in successive sessions. The same word lists are repeated
in sessions until the goal has been achieved for several sessions in a row. When
selecting words for these word lists, the use of high-frequency words within a
syllable category would increase the likelihood of generalization to text
reading
These same kinds of speed drills can be used for irregular words as well as
multisyllable words that incorporate higher-level concepts of structural
analysis (prefix, stem, and suffix). At the multi-syllable level, automatically
recognizing both the visual patterns related to syllable division as well as
prefixes, stems, and suffixes (larger chunks) can be very helpful. The SPIRE program contains
speed drills that at first mark these syllable breaks as well as prefixes and
suffixes and then fade the cues as the student progresses through the skill.
Not only do good readers
read fluently with adequate
speed, but
when they read
aloud, they also use
appropriate
phrasing, intonation, and
their
oral reading mirrors
their
spoken
language. |
Air writing - development of symbol imagery
Sky
writing, which involves using gross motor movements of the whole arm to form
letters in the air, has also been used to help reinforce single letter formation
by combining visual, auditory and tactile-kinesthetic cues. More recently an air
writing technique has been included as a component of a program intended to
improve symbol imagery, or the formation of orthographic representations (i.e.,
Seeing Stars Nanci Bell, 1997). This technique involves having the
student look at a word or word part pronounced by the teacher, name the letters,
and then use his finger to write the word in the air directly in his visual
field while looking at his finger. The student then reads the word from memory
and the teacher questions him about the order and placement of specific letters
in the word (e.g., "What is the third letter in the syllable?" "What is the
second letter?" etc.). The emphasis here is on enhancing the students' ability
to "see" the letter patterns in their minds.
TEXT LEVEL - FLUENCY
The lack of fluency in poor readers is evidenced by their slow, halting, and
inconsistent rate; poor phrasing; and inadequate intonation patterns. Not only
do good readers read fluently with adequate speed, but when they read aloud,
they also use appropriate phrasing, intonation, and their oral reading mirrors
their spoken language.
Fluency training helps a student to connect the prosody of spoken language to
the prosodic features of text that are signalled through punctuation. There are
features present in spoken language that provide clues to a speaker's intent
such as gestures, facial expression, intonation, and stress that are not present
in printed text. The absence of these prosodic features in text inhibits some
readers from chunking words (grouping by semantic and syntactic features) into
meaningful units. Just as we teach students to make sound-symbol correspondence
during decoding instruction, we also must teach readers to map the prosodic
features of spoken language onto the printed text. Structured and systematic
instruction in this area will facilitate spoken-to-print prosodic
correspondences and enhance comprehension.
Intonation/Punctuation
To begin to develop awareness of the prosodic features of language, teachers
can introduce a short three-word sentence with each of the three different words
underlined for stress (e.g. He is sick. He is sick. He is
sick. The teacher can then model the three sentences while discussing
the possible meaning for each variation. The students can practice reading them
with different stress until they are fluent. These simple three-word sentences
can be modified and expanded to include various verbs, pronouns, and tenses.
(e.g. You are sick. I am sick. They are sick.)
This strategy can also be used while increasing the length of phrases and
emphasizing the different meanings (e.g. Get out of bed. Get out of
bed. Get out of bed now. ) Teachers can also practice fluency
with common phrases that frequently occur in text. Prepositional phrases are
good syntactic structures for this type of work (e.g. on the _____, in the
_____, over the ________etc.).
Teachers can pair these printed phrases to oral intonational patterns that
include variations of rate, intensity, and pitch. Students can infer the
intended meaning as the teacher presents different prosodic variations of a
sentence. For example, when speakers want to stress a concept they often slow
their rate of speech and may speak in a louder voice (e.g. Joshua,
get-out-of-bed-NOW!). Often, the only text marker for this sentence
will be the exclamation point (!) but the speaker's intent will affect the
manner in which it is delivered. Practicing oral variations and then mapping the
prosodic features onto the text will assist students in making the connection
when reading.
This strategy can also be used to alert students to the prosodic features
present in punctuation marks. In the early stages using the alphabet helps to
focus a student on the punctuation marks without having to deal with meaning.
The teacher models for the students and then has them practice the combinations
using the correct intonational patterns to fit the punctuation mark (e.g., ABC.
DE? FGH! IJKL? or ABCD! EFGHI? KL.)
Short phrases with familiar words can be introduced through chunking
machines. A chunking machine is a tachistoscope that allows a student to pull
the reformatted or chunked text through the window to increase speed of
recognition. (See Figure 4 for an example of a chunking machine.)
Scooping
The incorporation of a multisensory component of scooping under syntactic
chunks may benefit some students as they read at the text level, where the
appropriate application of intonation and stress in conjunction with speed and
accuracy are considered primary. The following is a suggested progression for
repeated readings of a paragraph that incorporates systematic work at the phrase
and sentence levels:
Developing Anticipatory Set
As noted above, in addition to repeated readings and other sorts of speeded
practice, it has been suggested that fluency is enhanced by being able to
anticipate what is to come in the text, which in turn enhances comprehension.
Summary
Effective reading comprehension requires not only accurate reading skills but
also automatic and fluent reading ability. Many struggling readers have
difficulty moving to a level of automaticity and fluency that allows them to
easily comprehend what they are reading. We have discussed the underlying
processes involved in developing fluent reading as well as suggested some
techniques for improving fluency. Research in the area of developing accurate
decoding has consistently indicated that a systematic code based approach is
important for teaching beginning reading skills. The best techniques for
developing fluency, however, have not yet been clearly established. The
suggestions here are based on clinical experience and more systematic research
is needed to determine which methods or their components will be the most
efficient.
References
Adams, M.J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about
print. Cambridge, MA: MIT.
Chall, J. (1996). Learning to read: The
great debate (Third Edition). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Chall, J. (1983).
Stages of reading development. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Bell, N. (1997).
Seeing stars. San Luis Obispo, CA: Gander Educational
Publishing.
Clark-Edmands, S. (1998). Specialized program individualizing
reading excellence (SPIRE). Kennebunk, ME: Progress Learning,
Inc.
Fischer, P.E. (1994). Concept Phonics. Farmington, ME: Oxton
House.
Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In
Paterson, K.E., Marshall, J.C. and Coltheart, M. (Eds.), Surface dyslexia:
neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading. Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Gillingham, A. and Stillman, B.W. (1997).
Remedial training for children with specific disability in reading, spelling,
and penmanship. Cambridge, MA: Educators Publishing Service.
Greene,
J.F., and Woods, J.F. (1993). J and J language readers. Longmont, CO:
Sopris West.
Lyon, G.R. (1995). Towards a definition of dyslexia. Annals
of Dyslexia, 45:3-27.
National Reading Panel (2000). Teaching children to
read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on
reading and its implications for reading instruction. US Department of Health
and Human Services, National Institute of Child Health and Human Development,
NIH Pub. No. 00-4753.
Pearson, P.D. and Johnson, D.D. (1978). Teaching
Reading Comprehension. New York: Holt, Rhinehart, and Winston.
Sanders,
M. (2001). Understanding dyslexia and the reading process: A guide for
educators and parents. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Snow, C.E.,
Burns, M.S., and Griffin, P. (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young
children. Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press.
Steere, A., Peck, C.,
and Kahn, L. (1988). Solving language difficulties. Cambridge, MA:
Educators Publishing Service.
Raines, B.J. (1980). Alphabetic Phonics.
Cambridge, MA: Educators Publishing Service.
Torgesen, J.K., Rashotte, C.A.,
and Alexander, A.W. (2001). Principles of fluency instruction in reading:
Relationships with established empirical outcomes. In M. Wolf (Ed.),
Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain. Timonium, MD: York Press.
Wood,
F.B., Flowers, L., and Grigorenko, E. (2001). On the functional neuroanatomy of
fluency or why walking is just as important to reading as talking is. In M. Wolf
(Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain. Timonium: York
Press.
Additional Resources
Carreker, S. (1999). Teaching reading: Accurate decoding and fluency. In J.R.
Birsh (Ed.), Multisensory teaching of basic language skills. Baltimore,
MD: P.H. Brookes.
Honig, B., Diamond, L., and Gutlohn, L. (2000). Teaching
reading: Sourcebook for kindergarten through eighth grade. Novato, CA: Arena
Press.
Pamela E. Hook, Ph.D. is an Associate Professor in
Communication Sciences and Disorders at the Massachusetts General Hospital
Institute of Health Professions, a graduate-level training program in
speech-language pathology and reading. Her research interests include the
relationship between oral and written language, auditory processing and
nonverbal learning disabilities. She has been a consultant to schools, designing
language arts curriculum, training teachers, and working with parents. She also
designs computer software to teach phonemic awareness and reading
skills.
Sandra D. Jones, Ph.D. is the Program Coordinator for the Hanson
Initiative for Language and Literacy (HILL), a whole school model for
professional development, research and educational outreach that is affiliated
with the MGH Institute of Health Professions. She has conducted research in the
areas of auditory, visual, and cognitive intervention strategies for dyslexic
students. She has been a consultant to schools in the areas of literacy,
learning disabilities, behavior management, technology, strategic planning and
group facilitation techniques, and policy.
0 comments:
Post a Comment